Last week in our news literacy class, we had a great,
thought-provoking guest lecture by Dannagal Young. Young’s work is
part of a growing body of research providing evidence of the benefits of
watching satirical news programs such as The
Daily Show.
So far, we have evidence suggesting that Daily Show viewers vote more than
non-viewers, participate more in political campaigns, score higher on political
knowledge, and consume more news from other news sources. This seems to answer
the critics who worry that satirical news programs might lead to cynicism. But
perhaps it doesn’t.
There is an assumption that cynicism would manifest itself
as (or co-occur with) apathy. If The
Daily Show made viewers cynical, then we wouldn’t expect them to be more
civically engaged than non-viewers.
Cynicism or Being Principled?
I’d critique the assumption that cynicism’s connection to
apathy is the real worry. I’d define and measure cynicism as negativity about
something combined with an inability to
respond to evidence. So, would a liberal person acknowledge evidence that,
say, unions make countries or states less competitive, or would a conservative acknowledge
evidence that gun control laws dramatically reduce injury and deaths. Would they just say "you're misinterpreting the evidence" or "you're not looking at the evidence in proper context" or "you're ignoring this other important piece of evidence". An
inability to change one's view based on new evidence is, to me, a problem.
But other people (highly partisan people) might refer to
this orientation toward new evidence as “principled”: you think that unions are good no matter
what, military intervention is bad no matter what, corporate mergers are bad no
matter what, government regulation is bad no matter what. So, are you being
cynical or are you being principled? In either case, I suspect that exposure to certain kinds
of satire news (The Daily Show, or Rush Limbaugh, who could be understood or defended as a satirist) increases the frequency and intensity of this orientation. Meanwhile, websites that present
information on the same topics but do so through presenting evidence in a
more-or-less straight-faced manner, like fivethirtyeight, might decrease the frequency or intensity of this orientation.
Dannagal Young pointed out, rightly, that getting people to really consider evidence that contradicted existing beliefs would
be pretty hard. In some sense, we are “hard-wired” not to do such a thing.
There’s plenty of evidence to suggest that the resistance to certain kinds of
evidence is a matter of identity preservation. But it
seems worth considering the extent to which media can reinforce this instinct
to preserve one’s identity.
Angrier Candidates
Consider the difference between the rhetoric and reputation
of two candidates: Bill Clinton vs. Elizabeth Warren. Or, if you’d prefer to compare
conservatives: George W. Bush as a presidential candidate in 1999 vs. Ron Paul.
In both cases, the former candidate runs a campaign based on a kind of compassionate
rhetoric while the latter candidates run based on distrust and anger at established
authorities. Which of these candidates’ rhetoric resonates with voters? I’d
argue that the satire of Michael Moore and perhaps that of Jon Stewart cause
viewers to favor rhetoric that is based on anger against the status quo rather
than rhetoric of compassion. Is this necessarily bad? I’m not sure. But it
certainly doesn’t move us any closer to a world in which an informed electorate
is selecting a candidate based on evidence as to who might be the most
effective candidate. It’s just survival of the angriest.
Increasing
skepticism, but of whom?
As Young pointed out during her guest lecture, humor and
satire have been used to prompt audiences to question authorities since the
days of ancient Greece. But the humor also stops the audience from questioning
the arguments made by the comedian (or to view the evidence differently). Humor
prompts audiences to let their guard down and cultivate a kind of affinity for
the comedian and his.her views. Humor is an effective way to stop people from
questioning the authority of the comedian. It reduces people’s ability to counter-argue.
Again, will this help people select the best candidate? I think it just makes
people less likely to consider evidence that is inconsistent with the worldview
implicitly endorsed by the satirist.
Hard-wiring is not
fate
So, can media challenge our existing worldviews by
cultivating mindful consideration of the issues of the day? Last week, I hosted
a symposium on media choice at Drexel University. One of our guest speakers,
Talia Stroud, had an argument similar to Young’s about how we are “hard-wired”
to defend our ideological in-group or “tribe”. We are predisposed to political
or ideological polarization. The instinct to read other viewpoints and
arguments as somehow biased and flawed reflect deep-seated, automatic way of
acting and thinking. Stroud presented the results of years of research that, I
thought, reflected a terrific persistence and a refusal to accept hard-wired
predispositions as unchangeable. She presented the result of study after study
in which she and her colleagues tried to reduce ideological polarization when
people are exposed to viewpoints that differ from their own. Time and again,
the interventions not only didn’t work, but often made people more polarized! Finally, she had seen
evidence of reduced polarization when she presented news website users with a “respect”
button alongside the ubiquitous “like” button. When people are prompted to respect
a viewpoint other than their own along with the ability to endorse it through a
comment or a “like”, some of them will do so. This subtle change to websites
can affect discourse, and I think this change in discourse could potentially
reduce ideological polarization.
Even if a large number of people weren’t ever oriented
toward objectively considering evidence for or against a politically-charged
topic (e.g., man-made climate change, effects of reducing unemployment benefits)
in the past, are we to let this dictate what we’re capable of? Are we mistaking
predispositions for unchangeable fate? Are we not to even explore the
possibility of change? To do so would seem, well, cynical. And Stroud's research
shows that there is already evidence that simple tweaks to the media choice
environment cause changes in behavior and, perhaps, thinking, despite whatever hard-wiring we have.
No comments:
Post a Comment